Contents 1 Definition 2 Myths surrounding multilingualism 3 Multilingual individuals 3.1 Cognitive ability 3.2 Economic benefits 3.3 Receptive bilingualism 3.4 Personality 3.5 Terms 4 Learning language 5 Neuroscience 6 Psychology 7 Multilingualism within communities 8 Multilingualism between different language speakers 9 Multilingualism at the linguistic level 9.1 Models for native language literacy programs 9.2 Sequential model 9.3 Bilingual model 9.4 Coordinate model 9.5 Outcomes 10 Multilingualism in computing 10.1 Internet 11 Multilingualism in the workplace 11.1 Multilingualism in English speaking countries 11.2 Multilingualism in Asia 11.3 Multilingualism in Africa 11.4 Multilingualism in Europe 12 Multilingualism in music 13 See also 13.1 Policies and proposals 13.2 Education 13.3 Other 14 References 15 Further reading 16 External links

Definition[edit] This section does not cite any sources. Please help improve this section by adding citations to reliable sources. Unsourced material may be challenged and removed. (April 2013) (Learn how and when to remove this template message) The definition of multilingualism is a subject of debate in the very same way as the definition of language fluency. On one end of a sort of linguistic continuum, one may define multilingualism as complete competence and mastery in another language. The speaker would presumably have complete knowledge and control over the language so as to sound native. On the opposite end of the spectrum would be people who know enough phrases to get around as a tourist using the alternate language. Since 1992, Vivian Cook[6] has argued that most multilingual speakers fall somewhere between minimal and maximal definitions.[7] Cook calls these people multi-competent. In addition, there is no consistent definition of what constitutes a distinct language. For instance, scholars often disagree whether Scots is a language in its own right or a dialect of English.[8] Furthermore, what is considered a language can change, often for purely political purposes, such as when Serbo-Croatian was created as a standard language on the basis of the Eastern Herzegovinian dialect to function as umbrella for numerous South Slavic dialects, and after the breakup of Yugoslavia was split into Serbian, Croatian, Bosnian and Montenegrin, or when Ukrainian was dismissed as a Russian dialect by the Russian tsars to discourage national feelings.[9] Many small independent nations' schoolchildren are today compelled to learn multiple languages because of international interactions.[10] For example, in Finland, all children are required to learn at least two foreign languages: the other national language (Swedish or Finnish) and one alien language (usually English). Many Finnish schoolchildren also select further languages, such as German or Russian. In some large nations with multiple languages, such as India, schoolchildren may routinely learn multiple languages based on where they reside in the country. In major metropolitan areas of Central, Southern and Eastern India, many children may be fluent in four languages (the mother tongue, the state language, and the official languages of India, Hindi and English). Thus, a child of Telugu parents living in Bangalore will end up speaking his or her mother tongue (Telugu) at home and the state language (Kannada), Hindi and English in school and life.

Myths surrounding multilingualism[edit] Many myths and much prejudice has grown around the notions of bi- and multilingualism in some Western countries where monolingualism is the norm. Researchers from the UK and Poland have listed the most common misconceptions:[7] that bi- or multilinguals are exceptions to the ‘default’ monolingual ‘norm’; that in order to deserve the label ‘bi-/multilingual’, one needs to have an equal, ‘perfect’, ‘nativelike’ command of both/all of one's languages; that childhood bilingualism may be detrimental to both linguistic and cognitive development and consequently lead to poorer results at school; that exposing a child to more than one tongue may cause language impairment or deficits, or that for children already diagnosed with impairments two languages mean too much unnecessary pressure and effort; that the children do not have enough time to learn both languages, therefore it is better if they only acquire the majority language. that the children would be confused with having the ability to speak two languages and the “tip-of-the-tongue states” For instance, where one knows the meaning and the specific details of a word, but cannot retrieve a word. those bilingual individuals tend to have slightly fewer vocabularies and weaker in “verbal fluency tasks” than the monolingual counterpart These are all harmful convictions which have long been debunked,[7] yet still persist among many parents.

Multilingual individuals[edit] A multilingual person is someone who can communicate in more than one language, either actively (through speaking, writing, or signing) or passively (through listening, reading, or perceiving). More specifically, the terms bilingual and trilingual are used to describe comparable situations in which two or three languages are involved. A multilingual person is generally referred to as a polyglot. Multilingual speakers have acquired and maintained at least one language during childhood, the so-called first language (L1). The first language (sometimes also referred to as the mother tongue) is acquired without formal education, by mechanisms heavily disputed. Children acquiring two languages in this way are called simultaneous bilinguals. Even in the case of simultaneous bilinguals, one language usually dominates over the other.[11] In linguistics, first language acquisition is closely related to the concept of a "native speaker". According to a view widely held by linguists, a native speaker of a given language has in some respects a level of skill which a second (or subsequent) language learner cannot easily accomplish. Consequently, descriptive empirical studies of languages are usually carried out using only native speakers. This view is, however, slightly problematic, particularly as many non-native speakers demonstrably not only successfully engage with and in their non-native language societies, but in fact may become culturally and even linguistically important contributors (as, for example, writers, politicians, media personalities and performing artists) in their non-native language. In recent years, linguistic research has focused attention on the use of widely known world languages, such as English, as a lingua franca or a shared common language of professional and commercial communities. In lingua franca situations, most speakers of the common language are functionally multilingual. Cognitive ability[edit] Main article: Cognitive advantages of bilingualism People who know more than one language have been reported to be more adept at language learning compared to monolinguals.[5] Bilinguals who are highly proficient in two or more languages have been reported to have enhanced executive function or even have reduced-risk for dementia.[12][13][14][15] More recently, however, this claim has come under strong criticism[16][17] with repeated failures to replicate.[18][19] There is also a phenomenon known as distractive bilingualism or semilingualism. When acquisition of the first language is interrupted and insufficient or unstructured language input follows from the second language, as sometimes happens with immigrant children, the speaker can end up with two languages both mastered below the monolingual standard.[citation needed] A notable example can be found in the ethnic Bengali Muslim community of Assam province in India, hailing from East Bengal. Their mother tongue is Bengali, but they have no opportunity to study it in the school. Their medium language of study is Assamese, the provincial language. As a result, their predominant form of communication mixes the mother tongue and the medium language. Because they have no chance to study both the languages separately, they can't differentiate between the two or maintain such a difference in expression. Literacy plays an important role in the development of language in these immigrant children.[citation needed] Those who were literate in their first language before arriving, and who have support to maintain that literacy, are at the very least able to maintain and master their first language.[citation needed] There are differences between those who learn a language in a class environment and those who learn through total immersion, usually living in a country where the target language is widely spoken. Without the possibility to actively translate, due to a complete lack of any first language communication opportunity, the comparison between languages is reduced. The new language is almost independently learned, like the mother tongue for a child, with direct concept-to-language translation that can become more natural than word structures learned as a subject. Added to this, the uninterrupted, immediate and exclusive practice of the new language reinforces and deepens the attained knowledge. Economic benefits[edit] Bilinguals might have important labor market advantages over monolingual individuals as bilingual people are able to carry out duties that monolinguals cannot, such as interacting with customers who only speak a minority language. A study in Switzerland has found that multilingualism is positively correlated with an individual's salary, the productivity of firms, and the gross domestic production (GDP); the authors state that Switzerland's GDP is augmented by 10% by multilingualism.[20] A study in the United States by Agirdag found that bilingualism has substantial economic benefits as bilingual persons were found to have around $3,000 per year more salary than monolinguals.[21] Receptive bilingualism[edit] Main article: Passive speakers (language) Receptive bilinguals are those who have the ability to understand a second language but who cannot speak it or whose abilities to speak it are inhibited by psychological barriers. Receptive bilingualism is frequently encountered among adult immigrants to the U.S. who do not speak English as a native language but who have children who do speak English natively, usually in part because those children's education has been conducted in English; while the immigrant parents can understand both their native language and English, they speak only their native language to their children. If their children are likewise receptively bilingual but productively English-monolingual, throughout the conversation the parents will speak their native language and the children will speak English. If their children are productively bilingual, however, those children may answer in the parents' native language, in English, or in a combination of both languages, varying their choice of language depending on factors such as the communication's content, context, and/or emotional intensity and the presence or absence of third-party speakers of one language or the other. The third alternative represents the phenomenon of "code-switching" in which the productively bilingual party to a communication switches languages in the course of that communication. Receptively bilingual persons, especially children, may rapidly achieve oral fluency by spending extended time in situations where they are required to speak the language that they theretofore understood only passively. Until both generations achieve oral fluency, not all definitions of bilingualism accurately characterize the family as a whole, but the linguistic differences between the family's generations often constitute little or no impairment to the family's functionality.[citation needed] Receptive bilingualism in one language as exhibited by a speaker of another language, or even as exhibited by most speakers of that language, is not the same as mutual intelligibility of languages; the latter is a property of a pair of languages, namely a consequence of objectively high lexical and grammatical similarities between the languages themselves (e.g., Iberian Spanish and Iberian Portuguese), whereas the former is a property of one or more persons and is determined by subjective or intersubjective factors such as the respective languages' prevalence in the life history (including family upbringing, educational setting, and ambient culture) of the person or persons.[22] Personality[edit] Because it is difficult or impossible to master many of the high-level semantic aspects of a language (including but not limited to its idioms and eponyms) without first understanding the culture and history of the region in which that language evolved, as a practical matter an in-depth familiarity with multiple cultures is a prerequisite for high-level multilingualism. This knowledge of cultures individually and comparatively, or indeed the mere fact of one's having that knowledge, often forms an important part of both what one considers one's own personal identity to be and what others consider that identity to be.[23][24] Some studies have found that groups of multilingual individuals get higher average scores on tests for certain personality traits such as cultural empathy, open-mindedness and social initiative.[25][26] The idea of linguistic relativity, which claims that the language people speak influences the way they see the world, can be interpreted to mean that individuals who speak multiple languages have a broader, more diverse view of the world, even when speaking only one language at a time.[27] Some bilinguals feel that their personality changes depending on which language they are speaking;[28][29] thus multilingualism is said to create multiple personalities. Xiao-lei Wang states in her book Growing up with Three Languages: Birth to Eleven: "Languages used by speakers with one or more than one language are used not just to represent a unitary self, but to enact different kinds of selves, and different linguistic contexts create different kinds of self-expression and experiences for the same person." However, there has been little rigorous research done on this topic and it is difficult to define "personality" in this context. François Grosjean wrote: "What is seen as a change in personality is most probably simply a shift in attitudes and behaviors that correspond to a shift in situation or context, independent of language."[30] However, the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis, which states that a language shapes our vision of the world, may suggest that a language learned by a grown-up may have much fewer emotional connotations and therefore allow a more serene discussion than a language learned by a child and to that respect more or less bound to a child's perception of the world. A 2013 study published in PLoS ONE found that rather than an emotion-based explanation, switching into the second language seems to exempt bilinguals from the social norms and constraints such as political correctness.[31] Terms[edit] monolingual, monoglot - 1 language spoken bilingual, diglot - 2 languages spoken trilingual, triglot - 3 languages spoken quadrilingual, tetraglot - 4 languages spoken quinquelingual, pentaglot - 5 languages spoken sexalingual, hexaglot - 6 languages spoken septilingual, heptaglot - 7 languages spoken octolingual, octoglot - 8 languages spoken novelingual, enneaglot - 9 languages spoken decalingual, decaglot - 10 languages spoken undecalingual, hendecaglot - 11 languages spoken duodecalingual, dodecaglot - 12 languages spoken [32]

Learning language[edit] Main article: Language education One view is that of the linguist Noam Chomsky in what he calls the human language acquisition device—a mechanism which enables an individual to recreate correctly the rules and certain other characteristics of language used by speakers around the learner.[33] This device, according to Chomsky, wears out over time, and is not normally available by puberty, which he uses to explain the poor results some adolescents and adults have when learning aspects of a second language (L2). If language learning is a cognitive process, rather than a language acquisition device, as the school led by Stephen Krashen suggests, there would only be relative, not categorical, differences between the two types of language learning. Rod Ellis quotes research finding that the earlier children learn a second language, the better off they are, in terms of pronunciation. See Critical period hypothesis. European schools generally offer secondary language classes for their students early on, due to the interconnectedness with neighbour countries with different languages. Most European students now study at least two foreign languages, a process strongly encouraged by the European Union.[34] Based on the research in Ann Fathman's The Relationship between age and second language productive ability,[35][36] there is a difference in the rate of learning of English morphology, syntax and phonology based upon differences in age, but that the order of acquisition in second language learning does not change with age. In second language class, students will commonly face the difficulties on thinking in the target language because they are influenced by their native language and culture patterns. Robert B. Kaplan thinks that in second language classes, the foreign-student paper is out of focus because the foreign student is employing rhetoric and a sequence of thought which violate the expectations of the native reader.[37] Foreign students who have mastered syntactic structures have still demonstrated inability to compose adequate themes, term papers, theses, and dissertations. Robert B. Kaplan describes two key words that affect people when they learn a second language. Logic in the popular, rather than the logician's sense of the word, which is the basis of rhetoric, is evolved out of a culture; it is not universal. Rhetoric, then, is not universal either, but varies, from culture to culture and even from time to time within a given culture.[37] Language teachers know how to predict the differences between pronunciations or constructions in different languages, but they might be less clear about the differences between rhetoric, that is, in the way they use language to accomplish various purposes, particularly in writing.[38]

Neuroscience[edit] Main article: Neuroscience of multilingualism

Psychology[edit] A study in 2012 has shown that using a foreign language reduces decision-making biases. It was surmised that the framing effect disappeared when choices are presented in a foreign tongue. As human reasoning is shaped by two distinct modes of thought: one that is systematic, analytical and cognition-intensive, and another that is fast, unconscious and emotionally charged, it was believed that a second language provides a useful cognitive distance from automatic processes, promoting analytical thought and reducing unthinking, emotional reaction. Therefore, those who speak two languages have better critical thinking and decision making skills.[39] A study published a year later found that switching into the second language seems to exempt bilinguals from the social norms and constraints such as political correctness.[31] In 2014, another study has shown that people using a foreign language are more likely to make utilitarian decisions when faced with a moral dilemma, as in the trolley problem. The utilitarian option was chosen more often in the fat man case when presented in a foreign language. However, there was no difference in the switch track case. It was surmised that a foreign language lacks the emotional impact of one's native language.[40]

Multilingualism within communities[edit] Further information: List of multilingual countries and regions A bilingual sign in Brussels, the capital of Belgium. In Brussels, both Dutch and French are official languages. A multilingual sign at the Hong Kong-Macau Ferry Pier in Macau. At the top are Portuguese and Chinese, which are the official languages of Macau, while at the bottom are Japanese and English, which are common languages used by tourists (English is also one of Hong Kong's two official languages). A caution message in English, Kannada and Hindi found in Bangalore, India The three-language (Tamil, English and Hindi) name board at the Tirusulam suburban railway station in Chennai (Madras). Almost all railway stations in India have signs like these in three or more languages (English, Hindi and the local language). Multilingual sign at Vancouver International Airport, international arrivals area. Text in English, French, and Chinese is a permanent feature of this sign, while the right panel of the sign is a video screen that rotates through additional languages. Multilingual sign at an exit of SM Mall of Asia in Pasay City, Philippines. Three or four languages are shown: Japanese/Mandarin Chinese ("deguchi" or "chūkǒu", respectively), English ("exit") and Korean ("chulgu"). While Filipinos themselves are anglophone, such signs cater to the growing number of Koreans and other foreigners in the country. A Train name found in South India written in four languages: Kannada, Hindi, Tamil and English. Boards like this are common on trains which pass through two or more states where the languages spoken are different. A trilingual (Arabic, English and Urdu) sign in the UAE in the three widely spoken languages in the UAE Widespread multilingualism is one form of language contact. Multilingualism was more common in the past than is usually supposed[weasel words]: in early times, when most people were members of small language communities, it was necessary to know two or more languages for trade or any other dealings outside one's own town or village, and this holds good today in places of high linguistic diversity such as Sub-Saharan Africa and India. Linguist Ekkehard Wolff estimates that 50% of the population of Africa is multilingual.[41] In multilingual societies, not all speakers need to be multilingual. Some states can have multilingual policies and recognize several official languages, such as Canada (English and French). In some states, particular languages may be associated with particular regions in the state (e.g., Canada) or with particular ethnicities (e.g., Malaysia and Singapore). When all speakers are multilingual, linguists classify the community according to the functional distribution of the languages involved: Diglossia: if there is a structural functional distribution of the languages involved, the society is termed 'diglossic'. Typical diglossic areas are those areas in Europe where a regional language is used in informal, usually oral, contexts, while the state language is used in more formal situations. Frisia (with Frisian and German or Dutch) and Lusatia (with Sorbian and German) are well-known examples. Some writers limit diglossia to situations where the languages are closely related, and could be considered dialects of each other. This can also be observed in Scotland where, in formal situations, English is used. However, in informal situations in many areas, Scots is the preferred language of choice. A similar phenomenon is also observed in Arabic-speaking regions. The effects of diglossia could be seen in the difference between written Arabic (Modern Standard Arabic) and colloquial Arabic. However, as time goes, the Arabic language somewhere between the two have been created what some have deemed "Middle Arabic" or "Common Arabic". Because of this diversification of the language, the concept of spectroglossia[42] has been suggested. Ambilingualism: a region is called ambilingual if this functional distribution is not observed. In a typical ambilingual area it is nearly impossible to predict which language will be used in a given setting. True ambilingualism is rare. Ambilingual tendencies can be found in small states with multiple heritages like Luxembourg, which has a combined Franco-Germanic heritage, or Malaysia and Singapore, which fuses the cultures of Malays, China, and India or communities with high rates of deafness like Martha's Vineyard where historically most inhabitants spoke both MVSL and English or in southern Israel where locals speak both ABSL and either Arabic or Hebrew. Ambilingualism also can manifest in specific regions of larger states that have both a clearly dominant state language (be it de jure or de facto) and a protected minority language that is limited in terms of distribution of speakers within the country. This tendency is especially pronounced when, even though the local language is widely spoken, there is a reasonable assumption that all citizens speak the predominant state tongue (e.g., English in Quebec vs. Canada; Spanish in Catalonia vs. Spain). This phenomenon can also occur in border regions with many cross-border contacts. Bipart-lingualism: if more than one language can be heard in a small area, but the large majority of speakers are monolinguals, who have little contact with speakers from neighbouring ethnic groups, an area is called 'bipart-lingual'. An example of this is the Balkans. N.B. the terms given above all refer to situations describing only two languages. In cases of an unspecified number of languages, the terms polyglossia, omnilingualism, and multipart-lingualism are more appropriate.

Multilingualism between different language speakers[edit] Whenever two people meet, negotiations take place. If they want to express solidarity and sympathy, they tend to seek common features in their behavior. If speakers wish to express distance towards or even dislike of the person they are speaking to, the reverse is true, and differences are sought. This mechanism also extends to language, as described in the Communication Accommodation Theory. Some multilinguals use code-switching, a term that describes the process of 'swapping' between languages. In many cases, code-switching is motivated by the wish to express loyalty to more than one cultural group,[citation needed] as holds for many immigrant communities in the New World. Code-switching may also function as a strategy where proficiency is lacking. Such strategies are common if the vocabulary of one of the languages is not very elaborated for certain fields, or if the speakers have not developed proficiency in certain lexical domains, as in the case of immigrant languages. This code-switching appears in many forms. If a speaker has a positive attitude towards both languages and towards code-switching, many switches can be found, even within the same sentence.[43] If, however, the speaker is reluctant to use code-switching, as in the case of a lack of proficiency, he might knowingly or unknowingly try to camouflage his attempt by converting elements of one language into elements of the other language through calquing. This results in speakers using words like courrier noir (literally mail that is black) in French, instead of the proper word for blackmail, chantage. Sometimes a pidgin language may develop. A pidgin language is a fusion of two languages that is mutually understandable for both speakers. Some pidgin languages develop into real languages (such as Papiamento in Curaçao or Singlish in Singapore) while others remain as slangs or jargons (such as Helsinki slang, which is more or less mutually intelligible both in Finnish and Swedish).[clarification needed] In other cases, prolonged influence of languages on each other may have the effect of changing one or both to the point where it may be considered that a new language is born. For example, many linguists believe that the Occitan language and the Catalan language were formed because a population speaking a single Occitano-Romance language was divided into political spheres of influence of France and Spain, respectively. Yiddish is a complex blend of Middle High German with Hebrew and borrowings from Slavic languages. Bilingual interaction can even take place without the speakers switching. In certain areas, it is not uncommon for speakers each to use a different language within the same conversation. This phenomenon is found, amongst other places, in Scandinavia. Most speakers of Swedish, Norwegian and Danish can communicate with each other speaking their respective languages, while few can speak both (people used to these situations often adjust their language, avoiding words that are not found in the other language or that can be misunderstood). Using different languages is usually called non-convergent discourse, a term introduced by the Dutch linguist Reitze Jonkman. To a certain extent, this situation also exists between Dutch and Afrikaans, although everyday contact is fairly rare because of the distance between the two respective communities. Another example is the former state of Czechoslovakia, where two closely related and mutually intelligible languages (Czech and Slovak) were in common use. Most Czechs and Slovaks understand both languages, although they would use only one of them (their respective mother tongue) when speaking. For example, in Czechoslovakia it was common to hear two people talking on television each speaking a different language without any difficulty understanding each other. This bilinguality still exists nowadays, although it has started to deteriorate after Czechoslovakia split up.[citation needed] Japanese, English, and Russian sign in Northern Japan

Multilingualism at the linguistic level[edit] Models for native language literacy programs[edit] Sociopolitical as well as socio-cultural identity arguments may influence native language literacy. While these two camps may occupy much of the debate about which languages children will learn to read, a greater emphasis on the linguistic aspects of the argument is appropriate. In spite of the political turmoil precipitated by this debate, researchers continue to espouse a linguistic basis for it. This rationale is based upon the work of Jim Cummins (1983). Sequential model[edit] Main article: Sequential bilingualism In this model, learners receive literacy instruction in their native language until they acquire a "threshold" literacy proficiency. Some researchers use age 3 as the age when a child has basic communicative competence in their first language (Kessler, 1984).[44] Children may go through a process of sequential acquisition if they migrate at a young age to a country where a different language is spoken, or if the child exclusively speaks his or her heritage language at home until he/she is immersed in a school setting where instruction is offered in a different language. The phases children go through during sequential acquisition are less linear than for simultaneous acquisition and can vary greatly among children. Sequential acquisition is a more complex and lengthier process, although there is no indication that non-language-delayed children end up less proficient than simultaneous bilinguals, so long as they receive adequate input in both languages. Bilingual model[edit] Main article: Simultaneous bilingualism In this model, the native language and the community language are simultaneously taught. The advantage is literacy in two languages as the outcome. However, the teacher must be well-versed in both languages and also in techniques for teaching a second language. Coordinate model[edit] This model posits that equal time should be spent in separate instruction of the native language and of the community language. The native language class, however, focuses on basic literacy while the community language class focuses on listening and speaking skills. Being a bilingual does not necessarily mean that one can speak, for example, English and French. Outcomes[edit] Cummins' research concluded that the development of competence in the native language serves as a foundation of proficiency that can be transposed to the second language — the common underlying proficiency hypothesis. His work sought to overcome the perception propagated in the 1960s that learning two languages made for two competing aims. The belief was that the two languages were mutually exclusive and that learning a second required unlearning elements and dynamics of the first in order to accommodate the second (Hakuta, 1990). The evidence for this perspective relied on the fact that some errors in acquiring the second language were related to the rules of the first language (Hakuta, 1990). How this hypothesis holds under different types of languages such as Romance versus non-Western languages has yet to undergo research. Another new development that has influenced the linguistic argument for bilingual literacy is the length of time necessary to acquire the second language. While previously children were believed to have the ability to learn a language within a year, today researchers believe that within and across academic settings, the time span is nearer to five years (Collier, 1992; Ramirez, 1992). An interesting outcome of studies during the early 1990s however confirmed that students who do successfully complete bilingual instruction perform better academically (Collier, 1992; Ramirez, 1992). These students exhibit more cognitive elasticity including a better ability to analyse abstract visual patterns. Students who receive bidirectional bilingual instruction where equal proficiency in both languages is required perform at an even higher level. Examples of such programs include international and multi-national education schools.

Multilingualism in computing[edit] Dual language Hebrew and English keyboard With emerging markets and expanding international cooperation, business users expect to be able to use software and applications in their own language.[45] Multilingualisation (or "m17n", where "17" stands for 17 omitted letters) of computer systems can be considered part of a continuum between internationalization and localization: A localized system has been adapted or converted for a particular locale (other than the one it was originally developed for), including the language of the user interface, input, and display, and features such as time/date display and currency; but each instance of the system only supports a single locale. Multilingualised software supports multiple languages for display and input simultaneously, but generally has a single user interface language. Support for other locale features like time, date, number and currency formats may vary as the system tends towards full internationalisation. Generally a multilingualised system is intended for use in a specific locale, whilst allowing for multilingual content. An internationalised system is equipped for use in a range of locales, allowing for the co-existence of several languages and character sets in user interfaces and displays. In particular, a system may not be considered internationalised in the fullest sense unless the interface language is selectable by the user at runtime. Translating the user interface is usually part of the software localization process, which also includes adaptations such as units and date conversion. Many software applications are available in several languages, ranging from a handful (the most spoken languages) to dozens for the most popular applications (such as office suites, web browsers, etc.). Due to the status of English in computing, software development nearly always uses it (but see also Non-English-based programming languages), so almost all commercial software is initially available in an English version, and multilingual versions, if any, may be produced as alternative options based on the English original.[46] TheMultilingual App Toolkit (MAT)[47] was first released in concert with the release of Windows 8 as a way to provide developers a set of free tooling that enabled adding languages to their apps with just a few clicks, in large part due to the integration of a free, unlimited license to both the Microsoft Translator machine translation service and the Microsoft Language Platform service, along with platform extensibility to enable anyone to add translation services into MAT. Microsoft engineers and inventors of MAT, Jan A. Nelson and Camerum Lerum have continued to drive development of the tools, working with third parties and standards bodies to assure broad availability of multilingual app development is provided.[48] With the release of Windows 10, MAT is now delivering support for cross-platform development for Windows Universal Apps as well as IOS and Android. Internet[edit] Main article: Languages used on the Internet

Multilingualism in the workplace[edit] Globalization has led the world to be more deeply interconnected. Consequences of this are that more and more companies are trading with foreign countries, and with countries that don't necessarily speak the same language. English has become an important working knowledge mainly in multinational companies, but also in smaller companies. Multilingualism in English speaking countries[edit] According to Hewitt (2008) entrepreneurs in London from Poland, China or Turkey use English mainly for communication with customers, suppliers and banks, but their own native languages for work tasks and social purposes. Even in English speaking countries immigrants are still able to use their own mother tongue in the workplace thanks to other immigrants from the same place. Kovacs (2004)[49] describes this phenomenon in Australia with Finnish immigrants in the construction industry who spoke Finnish during working hours. But even though foreign languages may be used in the workplace, English is still a must-know working skill. Mainstream society justifies the divided job market, arguing that getting a low-paying job is the best newcomers can achieve considering their limited language skills. Multilingualism in Asia[edit] With companies going international they are now focusing more and more on the English level of their employees. Especially in South Korea since the 1990s, companies are using different English language testing to evaluate job applicants, and the criteria in those tests are constantly upgrading the level for good English. In India it is even possible to receive training to acquire an English accent, as the number of outsourced call centres in India has soared in the past decades. Meanwhile, Japan ranks 26th out of 63 countries in a 2014 English Proficiency Index, amid calls for this to improve in time for the 2020 Tokyo Olympics.[50] Within multiracial countries such as Malaysia and Singapore, it is not unusual for one to speak two or more languages, albeit with varying degrees of fluency.[51][52][53] Some are proficient in several Chinese dialects, given the linguistic diversity of the ethnic Chinese community in both countries. Multilingualism in Africa[edit] Not only in multinational companies is English an important skill, but also in the engineering industry, in the chemical, electrical and aeronautical fields. A study directed by Hill and van Zyl (2002) shows that in South Africa young black engineers used English most often for communication and documentation. However, Afrikaans and other local languages were also used to explain particular concepts to workers in order to ensure understanding and cooperation.[54] Multilingualism in Europe[edit] In Europe, as the domestic market is generally quite restricted, international trade is a norm. But there is no predominant language in Europe (with German spoken in Germany, Austria, Switzerland, Luxembourg, and Belgium; French in France, Belgium, Luxembourg, and Switzerland; and English in the United Kingdom, Ireland, and Malta). Most of the time English is used as a communication language, but in multilingual countries such as Belgium (Dutch, French and German), Switzerland (German, French, Italian and Romanche) or Spain (Spanish, Catalan, Basque and Galician) it is common to see employees mastering two or even three of those languages. Some languages such as Danish, Swedish and Norwegian or Croatian, Slovenian, Serbian and Bosnian are so close to each other that it is generally more common for meeting to use their mother tongue rather than English. Continued global diversity has led to an increasingly multilingual workforce. Europe has become an excellent model to observe this newly diversified labor culture. The expansion of the European Union with its open labor market has provided opportunities both for well-trained professionals and unskilled workers to move to new countries to seek employment. Political changes and turmoil have also led to migration and to the creation of new and more complex multilingual workplaces. In most wealthy and secure countries, immigrants are found mostly in low paid jobs but also, increasingly, in high status positions.[55]

Multilingualism in music[edit] Further information: List of multilingual bands and artists It is extremely common for music to be written in whatever the contemporary lingua franca is. If a song is not written in a common tongue, then it is usually written in whatever is the predominant language in the musician's country of origin, or in another widely recognized language, such as English, German, Spanish, or French.[citation needed] The bilingual song cycles "there..." and "Sing, Poetry" on the 2011 contemporary classical album Troika consist of musical settings of Russian poems with their English self-translations by Joseph Brodsky and Vladimir Nabokov, respectively.[56] Songs with lyrics in multiple languages are known as macaronic verse.

See also[edit] Polyglotism Cultural diversity List of multilingual countries and regions List of multilingual bands and artists Plurilingualism Code-switching Translanguaging Policies and proposals[edit] European Commissioner for Multilingualism European Day of Languages English-only movement Language legislation in Belgium Languages of Finland Languages of the European Union Multilingualism in Luxembourg Official bilingualism in Canada Education[edit] International Journal of Bilingualism Language education Multilingual Education Other[edit] Bilingual name Bilingual sign Childhood Bilingualism Research Centre (in Hong Kong) Economics of language Linguapax Prize List of artworks known in English by a foreign title Spanish language in the United States The Multilingual Library Category:Multilingual broadcasters

References[edit] ^ A Global Perspective on Bilingualism and Bilingual Education (1999), G. Richard Tucker, Carnegie Mellon University ^ "Europeans and their languages, a survey co-ordinated by the European Commission" (PDF). European Commission. Retrieved 2016-11-15.  ^ "The importance of multilingualism". Retrieved 2010-09-16.  ^ "Polyglot - definition of polyglot by the Free Online Dictionary, Thesaurus and Encyclopedia". Retrieved 2010-07-10.  ^ a b Kaushanskaya M, Marian V (2009). "The bilingual advantage in novel word learning". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 16 (4): 705–710. doi:10.3758/PBR.16.4.705.  ^ ^ a b c Paradowski MB, Bator A (2016). "Perceived effectiveness of language acquisition in the process of multilingual upbringing by parents of different nationalities". International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism: 1–19. doi:10.1080/13670050.2016.1203858.  ^ A.J. Aitken in The Oxford Companion to the English Language, Oxford University Press 1992. p.894 ^ Ems Ukaz ^ "Writing With English As A Second Language". Archived from the original on 21 April 2015.  ^ Hult, F.M. (2014). Covert bilingualism and symbolic competence: Analytical reflections on negotiating insider/outsider positionality in Swedish speech situations. Applied Linguistics, 35(1), 63-81. ^ Bialystok E, Martin MM (2004). "Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: evidence from the dimensional change card sort task". Dev Sci. 7 (3): 325–39. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00351.x. PMID 15595373.  ^ Bialystok E, Craik FI, Grady C, Chau W, Ishii R, Gunji A, Pantev C (2005). "Effect of bilingualism on cognitive control in the Simon task: evidence from MEG". NeuroImage. 24 (1): 40–49. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.044. PMID 15588595.  ^ Kluger, Jeffrey (18 July 2013). "How the Brain Benefits from Being Bilingual". TIME. Archived from the original on 21 July 2013.  ^ Atkinson AL (2016). "Does Bilingualism Delay the Development of Dementia?". Journal of European Psychology Students. 7 (1): 43–50. doi:10.5334/jeps.375. CS1 maint: Uses authors parameter (link) ^ Yong, Ed. "The Bitter Fight Over the Benefits of Bilingualism". The Atlantic. Retrieved 2016-02-11.  ^ Lehtonen, Minna; Soveri, Anna; Laine, Aini; Järvenpää, Janica; Bruin, Angela de; Antfolk, Jan (2018). "Is Bilingualism Associated With Enhanced Executive Functioning in Adults? A Meta-Analytic Review". Psychological Bulletin. 144 (3): 1–27. doi:10.1037/bul0000142.  ^ Bruin, Angela de; Treccani, Barbara; Sala, Sergio Della (2014-12-04). "Cognitive Advantage in Bilingualism An Example of Publication Bias?". Psychological Science. 26: 0956797614557866. doi:10.1177/0956797614557866. ISSN 0956-7976. PMID 25475825.  ^ Paap, Kenneth R.; Johnson, Hunter A.; Sawi, Oliver (2015-08-01). "Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances". Cortex. 69: 265–278. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014.  ^ Grin, François; Sfreddo, Claudio; Vaillancourt, François (2013). Economics of the multilingual workplace. [S.l.]: Routledge. ISBN 978-0-415-85106-0.  ^ Agirdag, O. (2014). "The long-term effects of bilingualism on children of immigration: student bilingualism and future earnings". International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism. 17 (4): 449–464. doi:10.1080/13670050.2013.816264.  ^ "Ethnologue report for language code: spa". Retrieved 2010-07-10.  ^ Halwachs, D.W. (1993). "Polysystem repertoire and identity". Grazer Linguistische Studien. 39–40: 71–90.  ^ Hult, F.M. (2014). Covert bilingualism and symbolic competence: Analytical reflections on negotiating insider/outsider positionality in Swedish speech situations. Applied Linguistics, 35(1), 63-81. ^ Dewaele, J. (2012). "Multilingualism, empathy, and multicompetence" (PDF). International Journal of Multilingualism: 1–15.  ^ Dewaele, J. (2007). "The effect of multilingualism, sociobiographical, and situational factors on communicative anxiety and foreign language anxiety of mature language learners". International Journal of Bilingualism. 11 (4): 391–409. doi:10.1177/13670069070110040301.  ^ Grosjean, F (2011). "Life as a bilingual: the reality of living with two or more languages". Psychology Today.  ^ Tokuhama-Espinosa, T. (2003). The multilingual mind: Issues discussed by, for, and about people living with many languages. Westport, Connecticut: Praeger Publishers.  ^ Wang, X. (2008). Growing up with three languages: Birth to eleven. Briston, United Kingdom: Multilingualism Matters.  ^ François Grosjean (author of chapter); Editor: I. Parasnis (1996). Living with two languages and two cultures, chapter in: Cultural and Language Diversity and the Deaf Experience. Cambridge University Press.  ^ a b Gawinkowska M, Paradowski MB, Bilewicz M (2013). "Second language as an exemptor from sociocultural norms. Emotion-Related Language Choice revisited". PLoS ONE. 8 (12): e8122. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0081225. PMC 3859501 . PMID 24349044.  ^ ^ Santrock, John W. (2008). Bilingualism and Second-Language Learning. A Topical Approach to Life-Span Development (4th ed.) (pp. 330–335). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. ^ EurActiv: Most EU students learn two foreign languages: Study, 28 September 2009, retrieved November 2011 ^ Fathman, Ann. The Relationship between age and second language productive ability. 27 October 2006 ^ "THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN AGE AND SECOND LANGUAGE PRODUCTIVE ABILITY". Language Learning. 25: 245–253. doi:10.1111/j.1467-1770.1975.tb00244.x. Retrieved 30 April 2016.  ^ a b Kaplan, Robert B. "Cultural thought patterns in inter-cultural education language learning". 16.1-2(2006). 1-20. Wiley Online Library. Web. 9 November 2010. ^ Gadda, George. Writing and Language Socialization Across Cultures: Some Implications for the classroom. Addison Wesley LongMan. Print. ^ Boaz Keysar; Sayuri L. Hayakawa; Sun Gyu An (April 18, 2012). "The Foreign-Language Effect : Thinking in a Foreign Tongue Reduces Decision Biases". Psychological Science. 23 (6): 661–668. doi:10.1177/0956797611432178. PMID 22517192.  ^ Albert Costa1, Alice Foucart, Sayuri Hayakawa, Melina Aparici, Jose Apesteguia, Joy Heafner, Boaz Keysar (April 23, 2014). "Your Morals Depend on Language". PLOS ONE. 9 (4): e94842. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0094842. PMC 3997430 . PMID 24760073. CS1 maint: Multiple names: authors list (link) ^ Wolff, Ekkehard (2000). Language and Society. In: Bernd Heine and Derek Nurse (Eds.) African Languages - An Introduction, 317. Cambridge University Press. ^ M.HBakalla(1984), Arabic Culture through its Language and Literature, Kegan Paul International, London ^ Poplack Shana (1980). "Sometimes I'll start a sentence in Spanish y termino en español": toward a typology of code-switching". Linguistics. 18 (7/8): 581–618. doi:10.1515/ling.1980.18.7-8.581.  ^ One Language or Two: Answers to Questions about Bilingualism in Language-Delayed Children ^ Dedić, N. and Stanier C., 2016., "An Evaluation of the Challenges of Multilingualism in Data Warehouse Development" in 18th International Conference on Enterprise Information Systems - ICEIS 2016, p. 196. ^ "Multilingual Functionality: New in Wolfram Language 11". Retrieved 2016-08-23.  ^ "The New Reynell Developmental Language Scales". Retrieved 21 Dec 2015.  ^ "Jan Nelson (Microsoft): The Multilingual App Toolkit Version 3.0". Retrieved 21 Dec 2015.  ^ Kovacs, M. (2004). Australian Finns on the verge of language shift. pp. 108, 200–223.  ^ The Japan Times 27 August 2015 ^ "9 of the world's most multilingual countries". Mother Nature Network. 28 June 2014.  ^ "A Model for Polyglossia and Multilingualism (With Special Reference to Singapore and Malaysia)". Language in Society. Cambridge University Press (digitally archived in JSTOR). 6 (3): 361–378. December 1977. doi:10.1017/s0047404500005066. JSTOR 4166945.  ^ Lim, Lisa. "English and Multilingualism in Singapore". Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics. Wiley Online.  ^ Gunnarson, Britt-Louise (2013). "Multilingualism in the Workplace". Annual Review of Applied Linguistics. 33: 162–189.  ^ Gunnarsson, Britt-Louise (2014). "Multilingualism in European Workplaces". Multilingua. 33: 11–33. doi:10.1515/multi-2014-0002.  ^ "Troika: Russia's westerly poetry in three orchestral song cycles", Rideau Rouge Records, ASIN: B005USB24A, 2011.

Further reading[edit] Bialystok, Ellen (2017). "The Bilingual Adaptation: How Minds Accommodate Experience" (PDF). American Psychological Association. 143 (3): 233-262. Retrieved 14 March 2017.  Bhatia, Tej; Ritchie, William C. (2004). The Handbook of Bilingualism. Oxford: Blackwell publishing. ISBN 978-0-631-22735-9.  Burck, Charlotte (2007). Multilingual Living. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan. ISBN 978-0-230-55433-7.  Jared Diamond, The World until Yesterday: What Can We Learn from Traditional Societies? (especially chapter ten: "Speaking in many tongues"), Penguin Books, 2012 (ISBN 978-0-141-02448-6). Komorowska, Hanna (2011). Issues in Promoting Multilingualism. Teaching – Learning – Assessment. Warsaw: Foundation for the Development of the Education System. ISBN 978-83-62634-19-4.  Kramsch, Claire (2011-06-03). "Are You Another Person When You Speak Another Language?". Berkeley Language Center. Retrieved 2013-06-02.  Romaine, Suzanne (1995). Bilingualism. Oxford: Blackwell. ISBN 978-0-631-19539-9.  Baker, Colin; Prys, Sylvia (1998). Encyclopedia of Bilingualism and Bilingual Education. ISBN 978-1853593628. 

External links[edit] Library resources about Multilingualism Resources in your library Resources in other libraries Look up multilingual, multilingualism, bilingual, or bilingualism in Wiktionary, the free dictionary. Media related to Multilingualism at Wikimedia Commons v t e Second-language acquisition General Outline Common misconceptions Learners Multilingualism Multiscriptualism Heritage language Multi-competence Learner language Contrastive analysis Contrastive rhetoric Error (linguistics) Error analysis Error treatment Fossilization Interlanguage Silent period Linguistic factors Language transfer Linguistic universal Word lists by frequency Individual variation Language-learning aptitude Critical period hypothesis Motivation Willingness to communicate Foreign language anxiety Metalinguistic awareness Strategies Language learning strategies Communication strategies Code-switching Good language learner studies SLA hypotheses Skill-based theories Order of acquisition Input hypothesis Interaction hypothesis Comprehensible output Competition model Connectionism Noticing hypothesis Processability theory Interface hypothesis Interface position Generative second-language acquisition Dynamic systems approach In the classroom Focus on form Input enhancement Aptitude tests MLAT DLAB PLAB CANAL-F Languages portal Authority control GND: 4038403-2 NDL: 00576834 Retrieved from "" Categories: Language acquisitionMultilingualismSociology of languageHidden categories: CS1 maint: Uses authors parameterCS1 maint: Multiple names: authors listArticles to be merged from February 2018All articles to be mergedUse dmy dates from August 2014Articles needing additional references from April 2013All articles needing additional referencesAll articles with unsourced statementsArticles with unsourced statements from July 2011Articles with unsourced statements from June 2013All articles with specifically marked weasel-worded phrasesArticles with specifically marked weasel-worded phrases from March 2009Articles with unsourced statements from November 2008Wikipedia articles needing clarification from July 2014Articles with unsourced statements from March 2008Articles with unsourced statements from October 2012Wikipedia articles with GND identifiers

Navigation menu Personal tools Not logged inTalkContributionsCreate accountLog in Namespaces ArticleTalk Variants Views ReadEditView history More Search Navigation Main pageContentsFeatured contentCurrent eventsRandom articleDonate to WikipediaWikipedia store Interaction HelpAbout WikipediaCommunity portalRecent changesContact page Tools What links hereRelated changesUpload fileSpecial pagesPermanent linkPage informationWikidata itemCite this page Print/export Create a bookDownload as PDFPrintable version In other projects Wikimedia Commons Languages العربيةAymar aruAzərbaycancaবাংলাБългарскиBrezhonegCatalàČeštinaDanskDeutschEestiΕλληνικάEspañolEsperantoEuskaraفارسیFrançaisGàidhligGalego한국어Հայերենहिन्दीHrvatskiBahasa IndonesiaÍslenskaItalianoעבריתLatinaLietuviųМакедонскиMaltiमराठीBahasa MelayuNederlands日本語NordfriiskNorskNorsk nynorskਪੰਜਾਬੀPapiamentuPolskiPortuguêsRomânăРусскийScotsසිංහලSimple EnglishSlovenčinaСрпски / srpskiSrpskohrvatski / српскохрватскиBasa SundaSuomiSvenskaதமிழ்తెలుగుትግርኛTürkçeУкраїнськаTiếng Việt粵語中文 Edit links This page was last edited on 3 March 2018, at 00:57. Text is available under the Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike License; additional terms may apply. By using this site, you agree to the Terms of Use and Privacy Policy. Wikipedia® is a registered trademark of the Wikimedia Foundation, Inc., a non-profit organization. Privacy policy About Wikipedia Disclaimers Contact Wikipedia Developers Cookie statement Mobile view (window.RLQ=window.RLQ||[]).push(function(){mw.config.set({"wgPageParseReport":{"limitreport":{"cputime":"0.656","walltime":"0.797","ppvisitednodes":{"value":4373,"limit":1000000},"ppgeneratednodes":{"value":0,"limit":1500000},"postexpandincludesize":{"value":128425,"limit":2097152},"templateargumentsize":{"value":6028,"limit":2097152},"expansiondepth":{"value":15,"limit":40},"expensivefunctioncount":{"value":11,"limit":500},"unstrip-depth":{"value":0,"limit":20},"unstrip-size":{"value":53049,"limit":5000000},"entityaccesscount":{"value":1,"limit":400},"timingprofile":["100.00% 655.637 1 -total"," 38.73% 253.960 1 Template:Reflist"," 14.76% 96.802 22 Template:Cite_journal"," 11.19% 73.373 7 Template:Citation_needed"," 10.73% 70.317 11 Template:Cite_web"," 9.80% 64.280 7 Template:Fix"," 9.38% 61.523 1 Template:Merge_from"," 6.32% 41.436 11 Template:Cite_book"," 5.35% 35.057 8 Template:Delink"," 5.27% 34.526 1 Template:Mbox"]},"scribunto":{"limitreport-timeusage":{"value":"0.302","limit":"10.000"},"limitreport-memusage":{"value":5989602,"limit":52428800}},"cachereport":{"origin":"mw1264","timestamp":"20180316141844","ttl":1900800,"transientcontent":false}}});});(window.RLQ=window.RLQ||[]).push(function(){mw.config.set({"wgBackendResponseTime":927,"wgHostname":"mw1264"});});

Multilingualism - Photos and All Basic Informations

Multilingualism More Links

PolyglotismWikipedia:MergingTalk:MultilingualismBilingual (disambiguation)Bilingualism (disambiguation)EnlargeMayorNovi SadSerbian LanguageHungarian LanguageSlovak LanguagePannonian Rusyn LanguageEnlargeSwitzerlandFederal Administration Of SwitzerlandLanguages Of SwitzerlandGerman LanguageFrench LanguageItalian LanguageRomansh LanguageLanguageMonolingualismEuropeansFirst LanguageMultiscriptualismPolyglotismFirst LanguageLanguage AcquisitionMinority LanguageInternationalization And LocalizationEnglish In ComputingNon-English-based Programming LanguagesWikipedia:Citing SourcesWikipedia:VerifiabilityHelp:Introduction To Referencing With Wiki Markup/1Wikipedia:VerifiabilityHelp:Maintenance Template RemovalVivian Cook (academic)Scots LanguageEnglish LanguageSerbo-Croatian LanguageStandard LanguageEastern Herzegovinian DialectDachspracheSouth Slavic LanguagesYugoslaviaSerbian LanguageCroatian LanguageBosnian LanguageMontenegrin LanguageUkrainian LanguageTsarPolyglotismFirst LanguageLingua FrancaCognitive Advantages Of BilingualismImmigrationWikipedia:Citation NeededBengali PeopleBangladeshBengali LanguageAssamese LanguageWikipedia:Citation NeededWikipedia:Citation NeededLanguage ImmersionPassive Speakers (language)Code-switchingWikipedia:Citation NeededMutual IntelligibilityIdiomEponymEmpathyOpen-mindednessLinguistic RelativityFrançois GrosjeanSapir-WhorfConnotationLanguage EducationLinguisticsNoam ChomskyLanguage Acquisition DevicePubertySecond LanguageCognitionStephen KrashenRod EllisPronunciationCritical Period HypothesisEuropean UnionSecond LanguageLogicRhetoricNeuroscience Of MultilingualismFraming Effect (psychology)Trolley ProblemList Of Multilingual Countries And RegionsEnlargeBrusselsBelgiumDutch LanguageFrench LanguageEnlargeMultilingual SignHong Kong-Macau Ferry Pier, MacauMacauPortuguese LanguageChinese LanguageOfficial LanguageJapanese LanguageEnglish LanguageTouristEnlargeEnglish LanguageKannada LanguageHindiBangaloreIndiaEnlargeTamil LanguageEnglish LanguageHindiChennaiEnlargeVancouver International AirportEnlargeSM Mall Of AsiaPasay CityPhilippinesJapanese LanguageMandarin ChineseEnglish Language In The PhilippinesKorean LanguageFilipino PeopleEnglish LanguageKoreans In The PhilippinesDemographics Of The PhilippinesEnlargeEnlargeArabic LanguageUrduUAELanguage ContactWikipedia:Avoid Weasel WordsSub-Saharan AfricaIndiaDiglossiaEuropeRegional LanguageFrisiaFrisian LanguagesGerman LanguageDutch LanguageLusatiaSorbian LanguagesEnglish LanguageScots LanguageModern Standard ArabicLuxembourgMalaysiaSingaporeEthnic MalaysChinaIndiaMartha's VineyardMartha's Vineyard Sign LanguageEnglish LanguageAl-Sayyid Bedouin Sign LanguageBalkansCommunication Accommodation TheoryCode-switchingWikipedia:Citation NeededCalqueBlackmailPidgin LanguagePapiamentoCuraçaoSinglishSingaporeHelsinki SlangMutual IntelligibilityWikipedia:Please ClarifyOccitan LanguageCatalan LanguageYiddish LanguageMiddle High GermanHebrewScandinaviaSwedish LanguageNorwegian LanguageDanish LanguageNon-convergent DiscourseNetherlandsDutch LanguageAfrikaans LanguageCzechoslovakiaCzech LanguageSlovak LanguageWikipedia:Citation NeededEnlargeSequential BilingualismSimultaneous BilingualismEnlargeHebrewInternationalization And LocalizationSoftware LocalizationList Of Languages By Number Of Native SpeakersOffice SuiteWeb BrowserEnglish In ComputingNon-English-based Programming LanguagesLanguages Used On The Internet2020 Tokyo OlympicsMalaysiaSingaporeList Of Territorial Entities Where German Is An Official LanguageList Of Territorial Entities Where French Is An Official LanguageList Of Territorial Entities Where English Is An Official LanguageDiversity (business)List Of Multilingual Bands And ArtistsLingua FrancaWikipedia:Citation NeededTroika (album)Joseph BrodskyVladimir NabokovMacaronic LanguagePolyglotismCultural DiversityList Of Multilingual Countries And RegionsList Of Multilingual Bands And ArtistsPlurilingualismCode-switchingTranslanguagingEuropean Commissioner For MultilingualismEuropean Day Of LanguagesEnglish-only MovementLanguage Legislation In BelgiumLanguages Of FinlandLanguages Of The European UnionMultilingualism In LuxembourgOfficial Bilingualism In CanadaInternational Journal Of BilingualismLanguage EducationMultilingual EducationBilingual NameBilingual SignChildhood Bilingualism Research CentreEconomics Of LanguageLinguapax PrizeList Of Artworks Known In English By A Foreign TitleSpanish Language In The United StatesThe Multilingual LibraryCategory:Multilingual BroadcastersDigital Object IdentifierDigital Object IdentifierA.J. AitkenEms UkazDigital Object IdentifierPubMed IdentifierDigital Object IdentifierPubMed IdentifierDigital Object IdentifierCategory:CS1 Maint: Uses Authors ParameterDigital Object IdentifierDigital Object IdentifierInternational Standard Serial NumberPubMed IdentifierDigital Object IdentifierInternational Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/978-0-415-85106-0Digital Object IdentifierDigital Object IdentifierDigital Object IdentifierPubMed CentralPubMed IdentifierDigital Object IdentifierDigital Object IdentifierPubMed IdentifierDigital Object IdentifierPubMed CentralPubMed IdentifierCategory:CS1 Maint: Multiple Names: Authors ListShana PoplackDigital Object IdentifierThe Japan TimesLanguage In SocietyCambridge University PressJSTORDigital Object IdentifierJSTORJohn Wiley & SonsDigital Object IdentifierInternational Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/978-0-631-22735-9International Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/978-0-230-55433-7Jared DiamondThe World Until YesterdayInternational Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/978-0-141-02448-6International Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/978-83-62634-19-4International Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/978-0-631-19539-9International Standard Book NumberSpecial:BookSources/978-1853593628Wikipedia:LIBRARYCommons:Category:MultilingualismTemplate:SLA TopicsTemplate Talk:SLA TopicsSecond-language AcquisitionOutline Of Second-language AcquisitionList Of Common Misconceptions About Language LearningMultiscriptualismHeritage Language LearningMulti-competenceContrastive AnalysisContrastive RhetoricError (linguistics)Error Analysis (linguistics)Error Treatment (linguistics)Interlanguage FossilizationInterlanguageSilent PeriodLanguage TransferLinguistic UniversalWord Lists By FrequencyIndividual Variation In Second Language AcquisitionLanguage-learning AptitudeCritical Period HypothesisMotivation In Second-language LearningWillingness To CommunicateForeign Language AnxietyMetalinguistic AwarenessLanguage Learning StrategiesCommunication Strategies In Second Language AcquisitionCode-switchingGood Language Learner StudiesTheories Of Second-language AcquisitionSkill-based Theories Of Second-language AcquisitionOrder Of AcquisitionInput HypothesisInteraction HypothesisComprehensible OutputCompetition ModelConnectionismNoticing HypothesisProcessability TheoryInterface HypothesisInterface PositionGenerative Second-language AcquisitionSecond Language DevelopmentSecond Language Acquisition Classroom ResearchFocus On FormInput EnhancementModern Language Aptitude TestDefense Language Aptitude BatteryPimsleur Language Aptitude BatteryCANAL-FPortal:LanguagesHelp:Authority ControlIntegrated Authority FileNational Diet LibraryHelp:CategoryCategory:Language AcquisitionCategory:MultilingualismCategory:Sociology Of LanguageCategory:CS1 Maint: Uses Authors ParameterCategory:CS1 Maint: Multiple Names: Authors ListCategory:Articles To Be Merged From February 2018Category:All Articles To Be MergedCategory:Use Dmy Dates From August 2014Category:Articles Needing Additional References From April 2013Category:All Articles Needing Additional ReferencesCategory:All Articles With Unsourced StatementsCategory:Articles With Unsourced Statements From July 2011Category:Articles With Unsourced Statements From June 2013Category:All Articles With Specifically Marked Weasel-worded PhrasesCategory:Articles With Specifically Marked Weasel-worded Phrases From March 2009Category:Articles With Unsourced Statements From November 2008Category:Wikipedia Articles Needing Clarification From July 2014Category:Articles With Unsourced Statements From March 2008Category:Articles With Unsourced Statements From October 2012Category:Wikipedia Articles With GND IdentifiersDiscussion About Edits From This IP Address [n]A List Of Edits Made From This IP Address [y]View The Content Page [c]Discussion About The Content Page [t]Edit This Page [e]Visit The Main Page [z]Guides To Browsing WikipediaFeatured Content – The Best Of WikipediaFind Background Information On Current EventsLoad A Random Article [x]Guidance On How To Use And Edit WikipediaFind Out About WikipediaAbout The Project, What You Can Do, Where To Find ThingsA List Of Recent Changes In The Wiki [r]List Of All English Wikipedia Pages Containing Links To This Page [j]Recent Changes In Pages Linked From This Page [k]Upload Files [u]A List Of All Special Pages [q]Wikipedia:AboutWikipedia:General Disclaimer

view link view link view link view link view link